logo ELT Concourse teacher training
Concourse 2

Intonation

bagpipes

intonation

Intonation is a complex area so this guide will only cover the most important aspects.  At the end of the guide there is a list of suggestions for further reading.


define

What is intonation?

A simple definition is that it refers to variations in spoken pitch or tone and stress or emphasis.
Intonation is often used to show the attitude of the speaker and signal such things as:

  1. The difference between statements and questions.  Some languages use only intonation to do this, having no grammatical difference between an affirmative and an interrogative sentence.
  2. The difference between various types of question such as those expecting a yes/no answer (closed questions) and those requiring elaboration such as questions beginning with why, who, what etc.
  3. The speaker's focus of attention on various bits of the message.

For example, if somebody asks you a simple yes/no question such as:
    Are you coming to the party?
how many ways can you vary the intonation on a simple 'yes' answer?
Look at the graphic above for some clues and try to express the following attitudes:

  1. No emotional response
  2. Positive confirmation
  3. Why do you ask?
  4. Of course!  I'm astonished you should ask!
  5. I'm not sure if I'm coming or not.  It depends.
  6. Carry on and tell me why you are asking.

Click here when you have done that.


test Now try this short test doing the same with the word Hello to see if you can match intonation pattern to speaker attitude.  Imagine you are answering the door or a telephone call.

If you got that mostly right, we can move on.  If you didn't, refresh this page and have another look.


tiger

Other languages

Intonation is a significant contributor to a foreign accent in learners of other languages because most adult learners will carry over the intonation patterns from their first languages into a language they are learning.
It is important to note here that intonation patterns, at least the conventional or canonical ones, will vary from language to language and cannot easily be transferred although, of course, some languages will share very similar patterns and that will make life somewhat easier for learners with those backgrounds.
It is also the case that intonation patterns will vary from dialect to dialect in most large languages and that, too, is a significant marker of the speaker's origin.
In French, for example, pitch rises towards the end of each clause and at the end of sentences which gives the language its recognisable intonational contour rather like:
french
whereas German, by contrast, exhibits a fall at the end of each cause and at the end of the sentence like this:
german
Italian speakers will tend to let the pitch fall on statements requiring an exclamation mark but speakers of Greek will do the opposite.
Question intonation is often, not always, signified by rising intonation and some languages have no other way to identify question forms at all.
In Mandarin Chinese, question and statement intonations follow similar contours but the question intonation starts, continues and finishes at a higher pitch level than a statement.
Russian speakers will increase pitch in alternative questions on both the first and second alternative (whereas English, for examples, lowers the pitch after the second alternative).
Within very large languages, such as Arabic, which is better considered as a language family rather than a single language, there are very significant differences between the intonation contours used in, say, Morocco, from those used in other Arabic-speaking cultures such as the Gulf States.


important

Why is intonation important?

Although this is undeniably a challenging area to teach, it is important for the simple reason that native speakers are accustomed to hearing English which contains errors in structure and the pronunciation of individual sounds (especially vowels) but may be wholly unaware that errors in intonation are even possible.  Wells puts it this way:

After all, almost any intonation pattern is possible in English; but different intonation patterns have different meanings.  The difficulty is that the pattern the learner uses may not have the meaning he or she intends.  Speakers of English assume that – when it comes to intonation – you mean what you say.  This may not be the same as what you think you are saying.
(Wells, 2006: 2)


key

Key concepts in intonation

Before we can analyse or attempt to teach intonation, there are four concepts to be understood.  They are:

  1. Pitch
  2. Stress
  3. Rhythm
  4. Tone groups

and we will take them one at a time.


piano

Pitch

the quality of a sound produced by the rate of vibrations

The physics

If you hit middle C on a piano, the sound you hear will be produced by a string vibrating 262 times a second.  That's usually expressed as 262 cps (cycles per second) or Hz (Hertz).  Making a piano do that is actually rather a complicated piece of engineering which relies on the fact that longer strings produce lower notes and thicker strings vibrate more slowly.  If you cut the string in half, and hit it again, the note will be exactly twice as high.

Your larynx, where you keep your vocal folds, is considerable smaller than the average piano so the effect is achieved by muscles working to tighten or relax the cords across which you are expelling air, lengthening and thickening them or shortening and narrowing them.  You may not be able to sing terribly well but you do have considerable control over the pitch of sounds that you produce.

The human ear can (in young people) detect sounds ranging from around 20 to 20,000 Hz and you can detect differences of pitch of around 2 or 3 Hz.  (Bats and elephants are a different matter.)  Given that the average range of human voice is around 160 Hz, that allows us to detect something like 65 variations.

Language pitch
We don't however, have 65 significant variations to contend with.  Authorities vary in this respect but the usual consensus is that significant pitch variations are confined to low, middle and high or sometimes as low, middle, high and extra high.
To imagine how that sounds try saying the word really at the four different pitches.  This may help:
really
On the left, you should sound non-committal, the, moving to the right, sound mildly surprised, very surprised and absolutely astonished respectively.  As you do so, your voice should start at a low pitch and move middle and high to extra high.
You do not have to screech because the differences between the pitches have only to be two or three hertz.  You will, however, need, probably, to increase the amount of energy you commit as you get to higher pitches showing that pitch and stress are intimately connected.


stress

Stress

giving particular emphasis or importance to a sound

Usually, stress is connected to loudness.  If you hit a piano key harder, it will produce a louder sound but one which has the same pitch.
In humans, as usual, the situation is a bit more complicated and there are two issues:

  1. If we make an extra effort to expel more air, we will certainly increase the loudness of the sound we produce.  However, the extra effort we make usually results in our also tightening (and shortening) the vocal cords and that, of course, will produce a heightened pitch as well.  This means that most stressed sounds are also at a higher pitch.
  2. At the same time, speakers usually also lengthen the sound when they wish to stress it.

This results in three variations, not one:

  1. Stressed sounds are louder
  2. Stressed sounds are longer
  3. Stressed sounds are at a higher pitch

Oddly, the evidence appears to be that loudness is not the deciding factor in what the hearer perceives – duration and pitch are more important.
Thankfully, most authorities concur that we only need to distinguish between stressed and unstressed sounds although there is clearly a whole range of intermediate possibilities with the three variables working together.


rhythm

Rhythm

the flow of words and phrases determined by the relation of long and short or stressed and unstressed syllables

Here is where we bump into a common division into syllable- and stress-timed languages.  The argument (based on Abercrombie, 1965) is that some languages (such as French) are syllable timed with each syllable taking the same amount of time to be uttered.  So we get:

In French:
french
in which all syllables take the same amount of time to utter and in English:
english
in which each section (called a foot) has one stressed syllable and any number of unstressed syllables, all of which take the same time to utter.

More recent work has somewhat refined the theory, placing languages on a cline from those which are strictly syllable timed to those which are strictly stress timed with gradations in between.  It is now widely accepted that to refer to a stress-timed or syllable-timed language is only a crude approximation of the truth.

Regional differences are also apparent within languages so, for example, Welsh speakers of English tend more towards syllable- than stress-timed production and South American speakers of Portuguese produce broadly more stress-timed language than European speakers of the language.
For more on this, including some consideration or Mora-timed languages, see the guide to sentence stress (new tab).


tone

The tone group

Descriptions of intonation are not concerned with the pitch of individual syllables but rather with pitch patterns or tunes.  The phonological unit set up to handle the structure of tunes is the tone group
(Brazil, Coulthard & Johns, 1980:6)

A common analogy used to describe tones is that they occur as ripples on the larger waves of intonation across the utterance.

The usual division of the tone group or unit is into four bits:
    (Prehead) (Head) Nucleus (Tail)

The reason that three of those bits are in brackets is that they may not always occur – the nucleus, however, is always present.
This is where things get complicated because there is less agreement among authorities in the analysis.

Nucleus
This is the key bit to focus on.  It is the syllable on which the main pitch change starts.  Some people call it the tonic syllable.
Although authorities differ, it's possible to distinguish 4 types of pitch movement on the nucleus: fall, rise, rise-fall and fall-rise.  Complicating the issue are the three or four pitch types recognised above (low, middle, high and extra high).  Combine all those and it's clear that there are 12 possible variations to consider.  The nucleus is followed by the ...
Tail
This, when it is present, continues the pitch movement which occurred in the Nucleus.  Some authorities don't even recognise its existence and combine it with the Nucleus or Tonic.
Prehead and Head
Together, these two constitute the syllables up to but not including the Nuclear syllable.  Some people don't even recognise two categories here and use Pretonic to describe the whole bit leading up to the Nucleus (or Tonic).  Usually, the pitch movement on this part of an utterance can be described in the same way as the pitch movement on the nucleus (fall, rise, rise-fall and fall-rise).  The movement, is, however, smoother and less emphatic.
There are those, Brazil et al, for example (1980:9), who suggest that the pitch movement on the Prehead and Head, while variable, is not actually significant.  That's convenient for teaching purposes because it means we can virtually ignore it and focus our learners on the pitch movements occurring on the Nucleus or Tonic syllable.

To repeat the data given at the outset, with some examples, 6 possible pitch changes on the Nucleus are suggested.  They are:

1 neutral arrow neutral tone showing little emotion; it may sound rude or uninterested:
flat
The whole phrase is the tone group
2 falling arrow falling tone showing a positive response:
fall
The fall occurs on right and that's the nucleus
3 rising arrow rising tone indicating slight surprise or a query:
rise
The rise occurs on true and that's the nucleus
4 sharp rising arrow sharply rising tone indicating astonishment or shock:
sharp rise
The sharp rise occurs on where and that's the nucleus
5 rise fall arrow rising tone followed by falling tone indicating doubt:
rise fall
The head (I) is where the rise begins and the main movement in tone is on might (the nucleus) with a falling tone on the tail (do)
6 fall rise arrow falling tone followed by rising tone indicating interest and desire for more:
fall risewarning
The head (ca) is where the fall begins and the main movement in tone is on rry (the nucleus) with a rising tone on the tail (on)
As was noted above, this pattern may also indicate a warning.

It bears repeating that we are exemplifying canonical (i.e., most usual and conventional) English intonation patterns here.
Languages vary significantly.


prosodic

Prosodic features: word and sentence stress

There are two key ideas here:

  1. Pitch or tone
    This refers to the 'note' of the voice being high, low or somewhere in the middle.  English uses tone as a component of intonation but other languages, called tonal languages, such as Mandarin or Thai, use tone to vary the meanings of words.  The classic example is the Mandarin word ma.  Spoken with a high tone, the word means mother, with a rising tone it means hemp, with a low fall-rise tone it means horse, and with a falling tone it means to scold.
  2. Stress
    This is a combination of loudness, duration and pitch or tone.  Some languages make great use of stress to distinguish lexical meaning.  For example, the Greek word πότε (with the stress on the first syllable [potay]) means when but the word ποτέ (with the stress on the second syllable [potay]) means never.  English rarely does this but there is a distinction between, for example, envelope and envelope.
    Usually, four types of stress in English are identified:
    1. Tonic stress: in any unit of intonation, the stress will always fall on a particular syllable but it will vary, usually falling towards the end of the unit, so we get:
          He's talking.
          He's talking to my
      brother.
          He's talking to my brother about
      football.
      The original stresses don't disappear in the first two sentences but become less stressed comparatively.  The assumed reason for this is that there is a clearly discernible tendency in English to place new information towards the end (or right) of an utterance.
    2. Emphatic stress occurs when the speaker wants to assign particular emphasis to a word or concept.  Often this focuses on a modal auxiliary verb or some kind of adverbial.  For example, compare:
          It was really hot vs. It was really hot
          He told me I must do it
      vs. He told me I must do it
    3. Contrastive stress is what we hear when the speaker wants to distinguish one concept from another.  So we get, e.g.
          They arrived together (not left together)
      vs.
          They arrived together (not separately).
      Contrastive stress is often called special stress.
    4. New information stress often occurs in questions, especially wh-questions, so we get a dialogue something like:
          Where did you go?
          I went to the cinema.
          What did you see?
          I saw that soppy love story.

      and so on.  Each question and each answer focuses the stress on the new information.
    5. Theme and rheme and stress
      The conventional sentence stress and the highest pitch and largest pitch movement occurs conventionally in English on the last content word (not function word) of the rheme of a sentence so, for example in:
          Because she wanted to meet his mother, she travelled up to London
      the greatest pitch movement (a rise-fall) occurs on the final part of the rheme, London.
      If we reverse the clauses, the pitch movement stays in the same place but, of course, it will now fall on mother because that is now the last content word in the rheme of the sentence.  It looks like this:
      contours
      So, reversing the clauses in a complex sentence like this involves making sure that the stress remains where it belongs.  Raising one clause or the other to the theme position also has communicative implications, of course, because the natural follow-on statement from the first example will concern London and from the second example, it will concern her mother.
      There is a little more on this in the guide to theme-rheme structures (new tab).

transcribe

Transcribing intonation

There are two ways generally in use:

  1. Using arrows such as ↑↓→ to show when the intonation changes and in which direction the pitch goes.
  2. Using contour lines such as contour to show the range across an utterance.

Additionally, the use of bold type or underlining, or both, as above, is common practice.  All these methods are used in this guide.


functions

The functions of intonation

The following is drawn from a key text by Wells,2006.  Wells has six essential functions (and we have added one).  The examples are not from Wells.

attitudinal function
smoking
This is the kind of thing we started with.  For example, if you say Good afternoon with a flat intonation, falling slightly at the end, you will probably sound like you are starting a serious business meeting and are keen to get on to the agenda.  It requires no response.
If you say it with a rise-fall intonation, falling on noon, after rising on after, you will sound more positive and welcoming.
It looks like this:
good afternoon
Attitudinal function reflects the emotional state of the speaker and may show anger, shock, sarcasm, irony and so on.  This is what is generally referred to as the tone of an utterance.
grammatical function
hat
We can make a question in English only by changing the intonation pattern.  So we can have:
questions
A number of languages (such as Greek) rely solely on intonation to make a question in spoken language.
Many would also claim that a question requiring a yes/no answer will have rising intonation along the sentence.  So, for example
    Are you coming to the meeting?
will rise on the word meeting to indicate that the speaker needs a yes/no response.
However, on a wh-question requiring more information from the listener, such as
    What time's the meeting?
the intonation falls on the final word.  The effect is something like:
question intonation
We also use tone to distinguish phrase constituents rather in the way that we use punctuation in written language.  So, for example:
    I spoke to the man behind the bar
can have two meanings depending on where the pause and tone changes occur.
If we have, e.g.:
    I spoke to the man [pause] behind the bar
we are stating that behind the bar is where I spoke to him but if we have:
    I spoke to the man behind the bar
with no pauses, then we are identifying the man by where he was.
Disambiguating which meaning is intended by pausing and tone shifts are what Wells refers to as the demarcative function of intonation.
This also occurs with relative pronoun clauses in English so the distinction between:
    The cats which were adopted by the old lady lived happily for many years
and
    The cats, which were adopted by the old lady, lived happily for many years
can be made by a falling tone and slight pause to signify the commas used in written texts.  The tone or pitch used on the non-defining relative clause will also often fall and the clause will be more quickly spoken than the rest of the sentence, signalling the fact that this is purely omissible and extra information rather than constituting part of the subject of the main verb.
This is also a discourse function and discussed again below.
focusing
focus
This occurs when the speaker wants to signal what is new and what is shared information.  For example, if we put a falling tone on the word pen in
    There's a ↓pen on the table
we are answering the question
    What's on the table?
but if we let the tone fall on table as in
    There's a pen on the table↓
we are answering the question
    Have you got something to write with? (or something similar).
It might look like this:
pen table
This is a form of markedness in some analyses and there is a guide to markedness on this site.
They important point is that we use tone to background some information and mark other information for emphasis, newness or importance.  Wells refers to this as a pragmatic function of intonation and states that it is perhaps the function most readily taught in the EFL classroom. (op cit:11)
discourse function
talk
We use intonation to show how ideas are connected.  Subordinate clauses, for example, have lower pitch and are spoken more quickly than the main clause.  So we can get:
umbrella
Another example, often asserted, is that intonation allows the hearer to distinguish between defining and non-defining pronoun relative clauses.  So we get:
relative clauses
signalling the commas in the second example by dropping the tone and speaking more quickly.  See the guide to relative pronoun clauses if this distinction is unclear to you.
The discourse function also plays a central role in turn-taking cues in interactions.  For more, see the guide to turn-taking.
disambiguation function
cat bird
This is an area allied to the last and is the one we have added.
We saw there that intonation, particularly with regard to where the nucleus of tone units occurs and what constitutes a separate tone unit is one way in which a defining relative clause can be distinguished from a non-defining one.  Otherwise, both would sound the same and it would lead to ambiguity in the mind of the hearer.
Here are some more examples of the way that intonation can disambiguate what are otherwise indistinguishable ideas:
  • I washed the car in the corner
    In this written sentence it is not possible to determine whether the car in question was the one in the corner or was washed there.  In spoken English, however, we can, if we are careful, express and detect the difference:
    We can phrase the sentence in two ways:
    1 I washed the CAR in the corner
    pre-head head nucleus tail
    2 I washed the car in the CORNER  
    pre-head head nucleus -
    Phrasing the clause in the first way determines that it is the car which was in the corner.  Phrasing it the second way signals that the washing happened in the corner.
  • I didn't go to the party because she might be there
    is another ambiguous statement when it is written and means either:
        I did not go to party at all and the reason for that was that she might be there
        I went to the party but not because she might be there

    Again, we can make the meaning clear by inserting a nuclear stress on the right element:
    1 I didn't go to the party because SHE might be there
    pre-head head nucleus tail
    2 I didn't go to the party BECAUSE she might be there
    pre-head head nucleus -
    In version 1, the meaning is that the speaker did not go for fear of meeting her.
    In version 2, the meaning is that the speaker went for a reason other than meeting her.
  • He didn't think the money had been stolen
    Here we have a rather unusual case because most people reading this would assume only one interpretation:
        His opinion was that the money had not been stolen.
    However, we can phrase and stress the sentence in two ways:
    1 - He didn't THINK the money had been stolen
    pre-head head nucleus tail
    2 He didn't think the money had been STOLEN
    pre-head head nucleus -
    and in the first case, we will not be surprised in the speaker went on with:
        He KNEW it had.
    One reason for the ambiguity is the tendency in English to transfer the negation from the logical place (the second clause) to the first clause.
    In most other languages, the thought would be expressed as:
        He thought the money hadn't been stolen
    in which there is no ambiguity.
There is a guide to ambiguity in general which considers this and other ways of disambiguating meaning, including intonation.
psychological function
masked
This refers to how we perceive sense units in utterances.  In lists, for example, we use something like
    I bought some ↑butter, some ↑jam, a loaf of ↑bread and a pint of ↓milk.
Here's another example of how pausing and the accompanying intonation is affected by the speaker's perception of sense units.  Compare:
  1. The people who under↓stood [PAUSE] quickly grasped the argument.
  2. The people who understood ↓quickly [PAUSE] grasped the argument.
In sentence 1., we know some people understood but the adverb quickly applies to the grasping of the point, not the understanding (which may have been slow).
In sentence 2., we know that people understood, too, but only the ones who quickly understood grasped the point.
The issue is to do with phrase constituents, to which there is a guide on this site.  In sentence 1., we have the verb phrase understood quickly and in sentence 2., the verb phrase is quickly grasped.
Strictly speaking, verb phrases consist only of verb forms rather than verbs plus adverbials but that does not matter for the purposes of this because adverbs are often embedded in verb phrases and the whole becomes a single sense unit.
Of course, the second sentence can be disambiguated in writing and speech by moving the adverb so we can have:
  1. The people who quickly understood grasped the argument.
  2. The people who understood grasped the argument quickly.
The psychological function also explains why we memorise strings by splitting them into tone units so, in English, a telephone number such as 284695677899 will be broken down as
    284↑-695↑-677↑-899↓
to make it possible (or at least easier) to remember.
indexical function
me
This refers to intonation acting as a marker of personal or social identity.  For example, newspaper sellers use a particular intonation pattern, as do newsreaders and people delivering lectures or sermons.  It has also been argued that the high-rise terminal, where all statements sound like questions with a rise at the end, is typical of certain social and age groups in Britain.  It has even been suggested that the tendency is caused by exposure to Australian television soap operas in which the pattern is common.
warning

Warning!

Do note imagine, however, that our reactions and attitudes are signalled only by the intonation we use.  Far from it.  If you or your materials imply anything of the sort, your students will be seriously misled.
This is what was said in the guide to key features of intonation and it bears repeating here:

There are no arguments for teaching intonation in terms of attitude, because the rules for use are too obscure, too amorphous, and too easily refutable.
(Brazil, Coulthard & Johns, 1980:120)

Intonation does play a communicative role, of course, but in conjunction with the setting, the roles of speaker and listener, their intentions and their shared information in the setting.


telephone

Intonation and communication

The communicative importance of all this is:

  1. where the speaker decides to place the Nucleus in an utterance and
  2. which of the 6 pitch changes is used

For example, consider the utterance:

She's going to see that film

Say this sentence aloud, doing three things:

and you'll soon hear the differences.
For example:

  1. Place the Nucleus on the first syllable (She's) and use a sharply rising pitch movement.  You'll get a sentence which sounds like an astonished question asking for the hearer to confirm its truth.
  2. Try again with the Nucleus on go: and a rise-fall, you'll get a sentence that sounds as if the speaker is confirming her fixed resolution to see the film.

Try doing those two and then writing down the way you see the intonation using arrows to show pitch movements and writing bits of the utterance above or below a base line to show pitch and then click here for a picture of how it might look.

key

Try a few more of these for yourself and identify how the sentence will sound to a listener.

This is the kind of thing which can be presented and practised but do not imagine, however, that our reactions and attitudes are signalled only by the intonation we use.  Far from it.  If you or your materials imply anything of the sort, your students will be seriously misled.  For example, the intonation in the first diagram above could just as easily show extreme anger as astonishment.
The point is:

There are no arguments for teaching intonation in terms of attitude, because the rules for use are too obscure, too amorphous, and too easily refutable.
Brazil, Coulthard & Johns, 1980:120

Intonation does play a communicative role, of course, but in conjunction with the setting, the roles of speaker and listener, their intentions and their shared information in the setting.


test Now you can try a short test of the key concepts.


A note on punctuation

The written form of intonation is punctuation although the resources available are much more limited.  Marks such as '!', '?' and even '??!!' as well as the use of dashes, commas, italics, bold type, UPPER-CASE LETTERS and full stops are all devices which can be used to represent spoken intonation.  They don't only do that, of course, as they are also involved in reducing ambiguity and making text coherent as well as cohesive.


test There's a summary test.



This is the index of other guides in the in-service pronunciation section.
the overview of pronunciation connected speech consonants
intonation minimal pairs (PDF) minimal pairs transcription test
sentence stress syllables and phonotactics teach yourself transcription
teaching pronunciation IP teaching troublesome sounds verb and noun inflexions IP
vowels word stress identifying word-stress IP
Guides marked IP are in the initial plus section.


References:
You may find some of the following helpful in this rather technical area.
Abercrombie. D, 1965, Studies in Phonetics and Linguistics, London, Oxford University Press
Brazil, D, 1975, Discourse Intonation, Birmingham: University of Birmingham: English Language Research
Brazil, D, Coulthard, RM and Johns, C, 1980, Discourse Intonation and Language Teaching, Harlow: Longman
Celce-Murcia, M, Brinton D and Goodwin J, 1996, Teaching Pronunciation, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Crystal, D, 1969, Prosodic Systems and Intonation in English, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Wells, JC, 2006, English Intonation: An Introduction, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press